The Size of Your Pupils While You Sleep Could Reveal The Memories You’re Reliving Sleep is often seen as the time when our bodies shut down and recharge, but recent research suggests that our brains are far from dormant. While we’re deep in slumber, our brains are hard at work, sorting, organizing, and processing memories. But how exactly does this process work? How do we manage to retain the new memories we've acquired during the day, while still holding on to long-term knowledge accumulated over years? The answer may lie in something we’ve all experienced—the dilation and constriction of our pupils. Thanks to groundbreaking research from Cornell University, scientists are starting to uncover the fascinating link between memory consolidation during sleep and pupil size. This work, using brain-scanning electrodes and eye-tracking cameras strapped to mice, shows that our pupils may actually reveal the process by which we distinguish between new memories and old ones while we sleep. Understanding Memory Processing During Sleep For years, neuroscientists have pondered the question of how new memories are integrated into our existing knowledge without erasing or blending old ones. After all, learning something new—like a foreign language or a new instrument—shouldn’t erase all the skills and information we’ve accumulated in the past, like how to ride a bike or tie our shoes. In this new study, researchers have uncovered a dual-phase system operating during the deepest stages of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, the stage most closely linked to memory formation and consolidation. These findings bring us one step closer to understanding how we can effectively learn without interfering with our prior knowledge. The study, published by a team from Cornell University, used a combination of brain-scanning electrodes and eye-tracking technology to observe mice as they learned new tasks (such as navigating a maze) during the day and slept at night. Remarkably, they observed that the size of the mice’s pupils varied depending on whether they were recalling new or old memories during sleep. NREM Sleep: The Critical Phase for Memory Formation During NREM sleep, which is considered the restorative phase of sleep, two distinct processes were observed. In one substage, when the mice were reliving or "replaying" their newly formed memories, their pupils constricted. In the other, when they were recalling older memories, the pupils dilated. These two phases occurred in rapid succession, which allowed the brain to distinguish and segregate new memories from older, more ingrained knowledge. What makes this discovery so exciting is that it sheds light on why learning new information doesn’t result in the erasure of what we’ve already learned. For example, learning to play a new song on the piano doesn’t mean you forget how to ride a bike. These findings reveal that the brain has a mechanism for keeping fresh memories distinct from older, more established knowledge. As the study's authors explain, “Our results suggest that the brain can multiplex distinct cognitive processes during sleep to facilitate continuous learning without interference.” The Role of Sharp-Wave Ripples in Memory Consolidation One of the key findings in the study was the identification of sharp-wave ripples (SWRs)—rapid electrical fluctuations in the brain’s hippocampus that are known to be involved in memory storage and consolidation. The team found that blocking these ripples during the stage when the pupils were constricted (and the brain was replaying new memories) prevented the mice from remembering anything new. This discovery provides further insight into how memory consolidation works, as well as the role these ripples play in encoding new memories. Azahara Oliva, one of the lead neuroscientists from Cornell University, likened the process to a cycle of alternating between "new learning" and "old knowledge" that happens slowly and methodically throughout sleep. “It’s like new learning, old knowledge, new learning, old knowledge, and that fluctuates slowly throughout the sleep,” she said. “We are proposing that the brain has this intermediate timescale that separates the new learning from the old knowledge.” Implications for Memory Research and Treatment These findings have far-reaching implications for the treatment of memory-related conditions, such as dementia or Alzheimer's disease, where the process of memory consolidation is disrupted. The ability to monitor and potentially influence these phases during sleep could lead to non-invasive therapies to either enhance memory retention or reduce cognitive decline. Moreover, these discoveries also lend weight to theories about how biological systems—like the human brain—could be vastly superior to artificial intelligence (AI) in terms of memory processing. In the realm of machine learning, the phenomenon known as "catastrophic forgetting" has long been a challenge. This occurs when AI systems, upon learning new information, forget previously learned data. The Cornell study's findings suggest that the brain may offer a potential solution for overcoming this issue, allowing for the integration of new information without sacrificing older knowledge. “This finding provides a potential solution for the long-standing problem in both biological and artificial neural networks of preventing catastrophic interference while also enabling memory integration,” the researchers wrote in their paper. What This Means for Future Research While the results of this study in mice are compelling, there is still much more to learn. Since humans and mice share many similarities in brain function, it’s likely that the same mechanisms are at play in our own sleep cycles. However, human studies will be necessary to confirm these findings and further explore how our pupils might serve as an indicator of memory consolidation during sleep. Furthermore, the ability to track pupil size non-invasively could serve as a tool for monitoring the brain’s processing of new and old memories. This technology could be used not just in clinical settings but also for enhancing learning techniques or developing better sleep-related interventions for individuals struggling with memory retention. Conclusion: The Complex World of Memory Consolidation This latest research has brought us closer to unraveling one of the most profound mysteries of the human brain: how we manage to store and integrate new memories while keeping our old ones intact. With pupil size providing a potential window into this complex process, the future of memory research seems poised for exciting breakthroughs. In the coming years, we may see new ways to improve memory retention, treat cognitive disorders, and even apply these principles to artificial intelligence systems. As we continue to explore the brain’s remarkable ability to multitask during sleep, one thing is certain: we’re just beginning to understand the full scope of how our minds work during the most restful time of the day.